Pronin Steele Ross.pdf

(379 KB) Pobierz
Journal of
Experimental
Social Psychology
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) 152–168
www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp
Identity bifurcation in response to stereotype threat:
Women and mathematics q
Emily Pronin, * Claude M. Steele, and Lee Ross
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, USA
Received 1 April 2002; revised 31 March 2003
Abstract
Three studies explored womens bifurcation of feminine identity as a response to threatening stereotypes in the domain of
mathematics. Study 1 demonstrated that women in a math class who previously had completed a large number of math courses
disavowed ‘‘feminine characteristics’’ strongly associated with stereotypes about womens potential for math success (e.g., flirta-
tiousness, planning to have children) but not characteristics perceived to be weakly associated with these stereotypes (e.g., empathy,
nurturance), more than women who had completed fewer math courses. Studies 2 and 3 directly manipulated stereotype threat by
presenting a scientific article reporting stereotype-consistent sex differences in math aptitude. As predicted, women strongly iden-
tified with mathematics responded to this threatening article by disavowing feminine characteristics strongly associated, but not
those weakly associated, with the relevant negative stereotypes, while women not strongly identified with mathematics showed no
such differentiation in response. Theoretical and practical implications of these results are discussed.
2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Perseverance in the face of group-based stereotypes
about ones limitations poses a daunting challenge. Be-
yond enduring negative expectations and discourage-
ment from others, members of the stereotyped group
may respond to inevitable disappointments and di-
culties by questioning their own fitness and acceptance
in the social environment. And they may be further
burdened by the knowledge that individual failures will
reinforce the negative views and assumptions held about
their group. The psychological threat associated with the
awareness that one may be viewed through the lens of a
negative stereotype has been termed stereotype threat
(Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995).
Such threat can be pernicious in its effects. In par-
ticular, there is evidence that it can impair performance
in a way that serves to validate the relevant stereotype.
Adverse effects of stereotype threat on individual per-
formance have been demonstrated for female students
undergoing tests of mathematical ability (Brown & Jo-
sephs, 1999; Davies, Spencer, Quinn, & Gerhradstein,
2001; Inzlicht & Ben-Zeev, 2000; Shih, Pittinsky, &
Ambady, 2000; Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999). Similar
effects have been found among black students under-
going standardized academic testing (Steele & Aronson,
1995), students from low socioeconomic backgrounds
undergoing purported ‘‘intellectual ability’’ testing
(Croizet & Claire, 1998), and even white men undergo-
ing a purported test of ‘‘natural athletic ability’’ (Stone,
Lynch, Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999). Moreover, there is
disturbing evidence that the negative consequences in
question are apt to be strongest among the very people
who have invested their self-worth in, tied their future
prospects to, or otherwise become strongly identified
with the relevant domain of achievement (Aronson,
Lustina, Keough, Steele, & Brown, 1999; Steele, 1997).
Previous researchers have explored a number of ego-
protective responses to stereotype threat. One well-
documented response is ‘‘disengagement’’ (Crocker,
Major, & Steele, 1998; Major, Spencer, Schmader,
Wolfe, & Crocker, 1998) or ‘‘disidentification’’ (Spencer
et al., 1999; Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995) with
respect to the domain in question. That is, the individual
q Research reported in this article was supported by grants from
the National Institute of Mental Health and the Russell Sage
Foundation. We gratefully acknowledge Christina Fuhrman, Kather-
ine Jakle, Peggy Lo, Dina Pugh, and Diana Tellefson for their help in
conducting this research.
*
Corresponding author. Current address: Psychology Department,
Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA.
E-mail address: epronin@princeton.edu (E. Pronin).
0022-1031/$ - see front matter 2003 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00088-X
965196542.013.png 965196542.014.png 965196542.015.png 965196542.016.png 965196542.001.png 965196542.002.png 965196542.003.png 965196542.004.png 965196542.005.png 965196542.006.png 965196542.007.png 965196542.008.png 965196542.009.png 965196542.010.png 965196542.011.png 965196542.012.png
 
E. Pronin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) 152–168
153
excludes performance in that domain as a basis for self-
evaluation, and may reject it as a basis of respect for
people in general (Crocker & Major, 1989). Indeed, the
individual may even foster an identity ‘‘oppositional’’ to
success in that domain (Ogbu, 1986).
Such ego-protective strategies are costly, both for the
individual and for his or her group. They are especially
costly when the domain in question is relevant to an
important avenue for professional or personal ad-
vancement, and when the individual has previously
demonstrated some aptitude or enjoyed some success in it.
Fortunately, disidentification is apt to be an ego-protec-
tive strategy of last resort. Rather than forfeit ones prior
‘‘investment’’ (not only of ego, but of time, training and
other resources as well), the individual in question is likely
to continue to strive for achievement in the domain and
seek alternative ways to deflect the relevant threat.
One such alternative involves disidentification not
from the domain of achievement in which the threat is
posed, but from the group targeted by the stereotype.
Again, this strategy can impose a heavy cost. Disiden-
tification with ones in-group and assimilation into some
out-group may oblige one to abandon previously valued
aspects of identity and sources of self-esteem including
aspects that are not ‘‘suspect’’ with respect to achieve-
ment in the relevant domain.
The present research focuses on a subtler and less
costly response to stereotype threat. Rather than dis-
identifying with a valued domain of achievement or
disidentifying globally with ones in-group, one can
disidentify selectively that is, disidentify with the as-
pects of ones in-group that are linked to disparagement
in that domain, while continuing to identify with valued
in-group characteristics that are not seen as linked to
such disparagement. We term this subtler response,
which might serve to deflect the relevant threat, identity
bifurcation. It is likely to occur when the benefits of such
domain identification in terms of the individuals sense
of self outweigh the costs of disavowing or abandoning
particular in-group characteristics especially while the
individual is present in the valued domain.
Our present contentions regarding identity bifurcation
derive in part from the literature on identity-oriented re-
sponses to group prejudice, primarily developed by social
identity theorists (Tajfel & Turner, 1986; also Ellemers,
1993; Hogg & Abrams, 1988). We combine two separate
principles described in this literature first, that individ-
uals aremotivated tomaintain group identities as a source
of positive self-evaluation, and second that individuals
are motivated to shed group identities that threaten po-
sitive self-evaluation. Our argument is essentially that
individuals tend to see their group identity not as an in-
divisible whole, but as a set of separable attributes (e.g.,
Biernat, Vescio, & Green, 1996; Reid & Deaux, 1996).
This ‘‘separability’’ allows the two motives we have noted
to be served simultaneously. That is, the individual re-
sponds to the threat of negative self-evaluation by dis-
identifying with ascribed group attributes that are viewed
as relevant to the negative stereotypes about the group (in
a particular domain), while maintaining or even
strengthening identification with attributes seen as ‘‘ste-
reotype-irrelevant.’’
To focus our immediate concern with math identifi-
cation and gender, consider the plight of a young woman
(whom well call Carol) who wins admission to an elite
college on the basis of the math and quantitative science
grades she achieved in high school, the recommendations
of her teachers, and her performance on standardized
tests. Once in college, she inevitably experiences the same
pressures faced by all prospective math majors who en-
counter more rigorous courses and stiffer competition
than they had faced in high school. However, Carol faces
additional burdens. She becomes increasingly aware that
characteristics commonly associated with her gender
(ranging from concern with hair and makeup to willing-
ness to consider future career interruptions in order to
have children) make her prospects as a successful math
major and futuremathematician suspect to her classmates
and to her professors. She soon recognizes that the in-
terest she evidences in her personal appearance and her
frankness in acknowledging the challenges of balancing
family and career concerns are seen as ‘‘counter-diag-
nostic’’ with respect to success in math-related fields.
When Carol walks into the math classroom, or into
her math professors oce hours, or chats with her math
study group, she feels heightened anxiety about the
prospect of negative judgment due to these aspects of
her female identity. To reduce this discomfort and
anxiety, she stops wearing makeup at least on days
when she has to meet with her professor or is likely to be
scrutinized by her male peers. When ‘‘hanging out’’ and
chatting with friends who belong to her math study
group, she avoids the topic of having children. However,
even at these moments, she continues to value her
overall identity as a woman, and to see herself as the
possessor of valued but stereotypically feminine char-
acteristics (such as social sensitivity) that she neither sees
nor expects her math peers to see as counter-diagnostic
of math success. Moreover, outside of such settings,
when her math ability is less under scrutiny and she is
less subject to stereotype threat, she may identify freely
with all of the feminine traits that she values, and even
with ones that she does not particularly value but nev-
ertheless acknowledges as an aspect of herself.
Gender stereotypes and mathematics
The present research focuses on female students re-
sponse to experiences in which they are exposed to the
threatening stereotype that women have less mathe-
matical ability than men. (Fully 45% of students in an
154
E. Pronin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) 152–168
introductory psychology class at Stanford, we should
note, reported holding this belief.) The dampening effect
of this stereotype on womens career aspirations and
choices, and thus on the representation of women both
in advanced quantitative courses and in professional
endeavors that demand quantitative skills (see Eccles,
1987) is all too apparent. For example, a report on the
faculty composition of the School of Science at the elite
Massachusetts Institute of Technology (1999) indicated
that, as of 1994, the faculty included 22 women, and 252
men. The reasons for such under-representation are of
course a subject for much debate, but in terms of our
present concerns it is important to note that women who
‘‘drop out’’ of quantitative majors do not tend to have
lower scores on college entrance exams, or lower fresh-
man grade-point averages than their male peers (e.g.,
Felder, Felder, Mauney, & Dietz, 1994; Seymour, 1995).
Womens experiences and peer interactions as math
students, and their sense of academic and personal
identity, thus merit close attention.
It is among women who major in quantitative fields or
who are otherwise ‘‘highly identified’’ with mathematics
(and who thus stand to be adversely affected by negative
evaluations) that we most expect to find identity bifur-
cation. It should be emphasized that we do not predict
that such math-identified women will adopt more mas-
culine traits in these contexts. What the identity bifurca-
tion hypothesis suggests is that women experiencing
stereotype threat in math will selectively disidentify
with stereotypically feminine traits that are linked to
negative stereotypes about their math ability or belonging
in the domain. Thus, we expect women under stereotype
threat to disidentify with characteristics viewed as in-
congruent with or compromising of math achievement
and belonging (e.g., wearing makeup) but not with other
characteristics viewed as less problematic for such
achievement and belonging (e.g., being empathic).
In focusing on the challenges faced by women in
quantitative fields, we are not, of course, suggesting that
women do not experience stereotype threat in other areas
of academic or professional achievement. Indeed, it is
apparent that in a variety of highly competitive achieve-
ment domains, from surgery (American College of Sur-
geons, 1996) to investment banking (The New York
Times, 1999), women find themselves in a distinct nu-
merical minority, and accordingly face negative stereo-
types and sources of stereotype threat that are likely to
evoke similar types of identity bifurcation. Our current
research is thus designed to offer an illustration of a
phenomenon that we believe is widespread in education
and in the workplace.
relying on our own intuitions about the relevant ste-
reotypes, we began with a preliminary study to identify
stereotypically feminine characteristics that our research
participants would view as strongly associated versus
weakly (or not at all) associated with negative stereo-
types about womens ability and potential in math. In
Study 1 we tested whether these trait classifications were
reflected in the differing self-descriptions, obtained spe-
cifically in the context of math class, of women who had
completed many versus few math courses. We then
conducted two laboratory studies to test the hypothesis
that experimentally induced stereotype threat would
increase identity bifurcation among women strong in
their identification with math.
Identification and validation of feminine characteristics
Our primary ethnographic source of information
about the relevant stereotypes for women in math was
Seymour and Hewitts (1997) extensive interviews with
women who were pursuing (or who had ‘‘dropped out’’
of) math-intensive academic majors. We also conducted
two 6-person focus groups at Stanford one with wo-
men majoring in, and one with women doing graduate
work in, quantitative areas. Both sources revealed fem-
inine characteristics viewed as relevant to the stereotypes
about womens math ability and potential for success.
One female engineering major, interviewed by Sey-
mour & Hewitt (S&H), described efforts to avoid a
stereotypically feminine appearance : ‘‘Theres no way Id
wear pantyhose, or mascara, or throw my hair up like
when I go to my art class’’ (p. 249). A Ph.D. student
from our focus groups echoed this concern, saying, ‘‘I
gave up wearing skirts [amongst my colleagues].’’ An-
other woman (S&H, p. 251) pointed out the danger of
flirting : ‘‘We get irritated with women who think theyre
gonna get by because they flirt with the faculty.’’ In our
focus groups, a graduate student expressed a similar
sentiment, saying she ‘‘would never flirt’’ in the context
of her quantitative classes. The perils of indulging in
gossip were also suggested by a student who said: ‘‘They
[male peers] are more intelligent and more serious,
whereas the women are more gossipy and chatty’’ (S&H,
p. 288). This concern was raised in our focus groups by
an undergraduate who reported preferring to do prob-
lem sets with men rather than women since ‘‘Guys dont
idle chit-chat.’’ Emotionality was seen as yet another
feminine trait that would raise suspicion perhaps be-
cause of its semantic opposition to rationality, a pre-
sumed hallmark of math aptitude. One graduate student
in our focus groups commented that ‘‘being emotional’’
would jeopardize her career efforts, noting specifically:
‘‘Tears do not earn you respect.’’ Perhaps most dis-
turbing was perceived conflict between the pursuit of
career success and expressed desire to have children (and
to spend time raising them). An engineering major (S&H,
Overview of studies
The present research sought to investigate identity
bifurcation in response to stereotype threat. Rather than
E. Pronin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) 152–168
155
p. 291) recounted that her engineering professor ‘‘didnt
think that women could have a job and raise a family at
the same time’’ (see also Etzkowitz, Kemelgor, Neus-
chatz, & Uzzi, 1992). A female graduate student from
our focus groups repeated this concern, claiming that a
woman would be negatively judged in her field for trying
to lead a ‘‘balanced life’’ involving work and family.
from work to raise children (M ¼ 4 : 62). The remaining
items, classified as low in stereotype relevance, were
sensitivity (M ¼ 3 : 75), being fashion conscious (M ¼
3 : 08), nurturance (M ¼ 2 : 94), and empathy (M ¼ 2 : 93).
Our empirical validation thus supported our a priori
analysis, and the resulting set of high stereotype-relevant
characteristics was, of course, seen as significantly more
likely to put a women at risk for negative judgment in
quantitative domains (M ¼ 4 : 97) than the set of char-
acteristics categorized as less relevant (M ¼ 3 : 18),
t ð 41 Þ¼ 12 : 39, p <: 0001. There was also a marginal
tendency (p ¼ : 08) for our English-class participants to
believe that the various feminine characteristics were, on
the whole, somewhat more likely to lead a woman to be
negatively judged in mathematics fields (M ¼ 4 : 46) than
did our math-class participants (M ¼ 4 : 06), but there
was no interaction effect between participant class (En-
glish vs. math) and characteristic type (high vs.
Preliminary study
To create the specific list of stereotypically feminine
characteristics that would be used in our studies, we
subjected a list of characteristics suggested by the Sey-
mour and Hewitt study and our focus group work to
empirical validation with an additional set of female
undergraduates.
Method
Female undergraduates completed a survey immedi-
ately after attending one of two intermediate level math
classes (n ¼ 22), or one of two English literature classes
(n ¼ 20). (They each received a large chocolate bar as a
reward for their efforts.) The survey called for ratings of
several ‘‘feminine’’ characteristics that the Seymour and
Hewitt study and our focus group work suggested were
incongruent with or seen as likely to compromise wo-
mens prospects for success in math-related fields (e.g.,
flirtatiousness, tendency to gossip), and also some char-
acteristics that those sources had not cited and were
presumably less relevant to or less incongruent with such
success (e.g., sensitivity, nurturance). Participants were
asked to ‘‘rate how much you think each of these
characteristics would put a woman at risk for being
negatively judged in quantitatively based fields and/or
careers’’ (1 ¼ Low risk for being negatively judged,
7 ¼ High risk for being negatively judged).
low
stereotype relevant), F < 1.
Our efforts to identify and validate two sets of femi-
nine characteristics that differed in the degree to which
they pose a threat to prospects for success and positive
regard in math-related fields were thus successful. We
cannot claim, and do not imagine, that our modest
undertaking exhausted the relevant categories. There are
no doubt other ‘‘feminine’’ traits that raters would deem
incongruent with success in math endeavors, and many
that they would deem irrelevant to such success. Our
present contention is simply that our two sets of char-
acteristics provided us with the means to test our bi-
furcation hypothesis in the studies that we now report.
It is worth emphasizing that none of the characteristics
on either our ‘‘high’’ or ‘‘low’’ stereotype-relevant lists
can reasonably be thought to attenuate math skills in any
direct fashion. Rather, the qualities on the former list, but
not those on the latter, seem incongruent with the image,
and thus the identity, of math majors. Thus, it is the
qualities on the former list from which we predict that
math-identified women will disidentify regardless of
whether they (or others) believe that these characteristics
actually compromise ability or harm performance.
With this empirically derived dichotomy of feminine
characteristics in hand, we next sought to determine
whether women who had taken more math classes than
their peers would describe themselves in ways that con-
firmed the psychological significance of this dichotomy.
Results and discussion
In establishing our two lists of feminine characteris-
tics for subsequent use, we used the midpoint of the
relevant 7-point rating scale as our criterion. Charac-
teristics that received mean ratings above the midpoint
(i.e., those assessed to have moderate or high probability
of causing a woman to be negatively stereotyped) were
categorized as high in stereotype relevance, and char-
acteristics that received mean ratings below the mid-
point (i.e., those assessed to have low or no probability
of producing those consequences) were categorized as
low in stereotype relevance.
In all cases, our raters assessments proved consistent
with the evidence from the Seymour and Hewitt study
and our own focus groups. The items thus classified as
high in stereotype relevance included the wearing of
makeup (M ¼ 5 : 01), flirtatiousness (M ¼ 5 : 08), being
gossipy (M ¼ 5 : 51), emotionality (M ¼ 5 : 55), wanting
children (M ¼ 4 : 02), and planning to spend time away
Study 1
Our first study simply sought to determine whether
the pattern of identity bifurcation we postulated would
be more apparent among women attending math class
who had spent a lot of time in quantitative courses, and
presumably been heavily exposed to the relevant sources
of stereotype threat, than among women attending the
156
E. Pronin et al. / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40 (2004) 152–168
same classes who had spent less time in such courses and
presumably been less heavily exposed to the relevant
stereotype threat. While the results of such a study
would be correlational, and thus could not prove that
the predicted pattern of identification was caused by
stereotype threat, we anticipated that our results would
serve both to pave the way for our later experiments and
to offer external validity for the division of high vs. low
stereotype-relevance characteristics that we would rely
on in those experiments.
In this study, math students rated their identification
with various characteristics, including gender-stereo-
typic ones. We deliberately had them complete the sur-
vey in math class, since we deemed it especially likely
that the relevant pattern of selective disidentification
would be present in that context.
The ‘‘feminine’’ characteristics classified as high in
stereotype relevance (in italics below) or low in stereo-
type relevance (underlined below) were interspersed
among various ‘‘masculine’’ and ‘‘gender-neutral’’ items
in a larger survey, titled ‘‘Survey of Stanford Students.’’
The survey consisted of five sections, each with its own
heading (and separate items): (1) Personality (competi-
tive, sensitive , adaptable, flirtatious, adventurous, emo-
tional, friendly, nurturing , analytical), (2) Interpersonal
Style (extroverted, assertive, gossipy, empathic , aggres-
sive, talk about politics, talk about relationships,
reliable), (3) Appearance (physically fit, fashionable ,
laid-back, style hair, wear makeup), (4) Activities
(hanging out with friends, playing sports, shopping,
reading, engaging in artistic activities, talking on the
telephone), and (5) Future Plans (graduate school, full-
time job, marriage, having children, leaving work to raise
children, having a satisfying career). 1
Gender and course exposure information were col-
lected on a page entitled ‘‘Background Information’’
appended to the survey. On this page, participants indi-
cated their year in school, gender, age, ethnicity, US or
foreign citizenship, and current or intended major. They
then listed the quantitative courses they had taken in high
school and college (including mathematics at the pre-
calculus level or beyond, as well as courses in the physical
sciences, computer science, engineering, and statistics).
Method
Participants
Participants were 51 female (and an additional 103
male) Stanford undergraduates in 7 math classes (92% of
those attending). Two of the classes were the final course
in an introductory-level 3-course sequence, 3 were the fi-
nal course in an intermediate-level 3-course sequence, and
2 were single term intermediate-level classes.
Procedure
At the end of his or her lecture, the professor an-
nounced that students were being asked to stay for a few
minutes after class to complete a survey. The experi-
menter then introduced herself as a graduate student in
the department of psychology, told students that their
help in completing the survey would be appreciated (and
compensated with a chocolate bar), offered to answer
any questions about the survey once responses were
collected, and distributed the survey.
Results
The median number of quantitative courses taken by
the women in our sample was 10. Accordingly, women
were classified as high versus low in their exposure to
math depending on whether they reported having taken
more than 10 quantitative courses (n ¼ 24) or 10 or
fewer quantitative courses (n ¼ 27). The same criterion
was used for dividing the men in our sample into high
(n ¼ 72) and low (n ¼ 31) math exposure groups.
Our main prediction was that women who had
completed a relatively large number of math-intensive
classes would show the predicted pattern of bifurcated
gender identity to a greater degree than women who had
complete fewer courses, while we expected no such dif-
ferentiation among men. Thus, we predicted a 2 (math
class exposure: high vs. low) 2 (type of feminine
characteristic: high stereotype relevance vs. low stereo-
Measures
Participants ratings of their identification with specific
‘‘feminine’’ characteristics constituted our dependent
measure. Items were modeled after the self-schema mea-
sure developed by Markus (1977). That is, participants
first rated how much each characteristic applied to them,
and then how important having that characteristic was to
their sense of self. Consistent with methodology devel-
oped by Markus (see also Brown, 1998), these two re-
sponses were averaged together to obtain a single mean
representation of the extent to which a given character-
istic was part of the individuals ‘‘self-schema,’’ or iden-
tity. For example, they first were asked, ‘‘How sensitive
are you?’’ (1 ¼ not at all sensitive,7 ¼ very sensitive), and
then, ‘‘How important is being sensitive to you?’’ (1 ¼ not
at all important,7 ¼ very important). These two types of
responses, as in earlier studies, showed a high degree of
internal consistency, Cronbachs a ¼ : 91.
1 This study was conducted before the study that served to validate
the composites of more vs. less ‘‘stereotype relevant’’ feminine
characteristics. At the time it was conducted, we anticipated that
talking about relationships, shopping, and talking on the telephone
would be viewed as non-relevant characteristics, and that styling one s
hair would be viewed as relevant. Because our validation study did not
support our a priori classification of these items, they are excluded
from the analyses reported here and from our subsequent studies.
(Including these items, it should be noted, would not change the
statistical significance of the relevant findings.)
 
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin